Working memory reaches its peak in the 30s.
Working memory across the lifespan: A cross-sectional approach
(PDF) Working memory across the lifespan: A cross-sectional approach
o3.iconThis study is a representative basic study in our field, simultaneously examining the developmental curve and structure of working memory (WM) across ages 5-80 years using a large cross-sectional data set of N = 1 070 individuals.
1 Background and Objectives
The purpose of this study was to compensate for the fact that conventional developmental research has been biased toward children and adolescents and lacks data on older age. (ResearchGate)
The research questions are (1) the amount of change in WM levels with development and aging, and (2) examining the structural controversy of whether WM is "function-specific (retention vs. manipulation)" or "domain-specific (language vs. space)". (Taylor & Francis Online, ResearchGate https ://www.researchgate.net/publication/279417312_Working_Memory_Across_the_Adult_Lifespan?utm_source=chatgpt.com)
2 Participants and Methods
2.1 Samples
The general population of 1070 persons aged 5-80 years (almost equally divided between men and women) was stratified into 8 age groups (5-6, 7-9, 10-12, 13-19, 20-29, 30-39, 40-59, 60-80 30-39, 40-59, 60-80 years). ([ResearchGate https://www.researchgate.net/publication/271673579_Working_memory_across_the_lifespan_A_cross-sectional_approach?utm _source=chatgpt.com], UNF Digital Commons)
2.2 Measurement
Six tasks were extracted from the Automated Working Memory Assessment (AWMA),
Language WM: Digit Recall (retention), Non-word Recall, Listening Recall (manipulation)
Visuospatial WM: Dot Matrix (retention), Spatial Span, Visual Patterns (manipulation)
All assignments converted to standard scores (M = 100, SD = 15). (ResearchGate, UNF Digital Commons)
Differences between age bands and factor structure were tested with a multigroup covariance structure analysis (CFA), comparing three models (1 factor / 2 factors of function / 2 factors of domain). (ResearchGate)
3 Main Results
https://gyazo.com/fb85fd3b91db3ebcc984611fc6ff5d88
Language WM was maintained in old age, with 70-80 year olds comparable to teenagers, whereas visuospatial WM declined to pediatric levels. (ResearchGate, ScienceDirect)
A much different story spreading on social networking sites.nishio.icon
The only part of the article that is presented in a radical way is "visuospatial WM has been reduced to the level of a child."
Interesting that the language WM is maintained.
3.2 Factor Structure
CFA Conformance:.
Domain segmentation model (2 factors for language and visuospatial) best fit (CFI = .98, RMSEA ≈ .14). (ResearchGate)
Functional categories (retention/operation) are not supported, suggesting that "which area is more decisive than which operation". (ResearchGate, Frontiers)
3.3 Additional Findings
Covariates such as gender and years of education have no statistically significant effect, and age and domain are the main effects. ([scholars.unf.edu https://scholars.unf.edu/en/publications/working-memory-across-the-lifespan-a-cross-sectional-approach?utm_ source=chatgpt.com])
4 Significance
1. reassessment of aging tolerance
The pattern of protection of language WM over visuospatial is consistent with the neuroscientific finding of a left-hemispheric dominant "language network resistant to aging". (ResearchGate, PMC)
2. education and intervention design
Supported the significance of incorporating WM training during the rapid growth phase of childhood, and suggested that the "visuospatial system" should be reinforced in older adults' interventions. (Taylor & Francis Online, SpringerOpen https ://ejo.springeropen.com/articles/10.1186/s43163-025-00806-6?utm_source=chatgpt.com)
3. contribution to the model controversy
It strengthened the view of WM as "language + visuospatial resources" rather than "capacity + processing" and served as the theoretical foundation for subsequent network analysis research. (Frontiers, ScienceDirect)
5 Limitations and Criticisms
table:_
Item Content
Design Cross-sectional study, so intra-individual variation cannot be tracked, and the inclusion of generational effects cannot be ruled out. (ResearchGate)
Issue Selection Restricted to generalization between variants because WM measures other than AWMA and processing speed and IQ are not included. (ResearchGate)
Smaller number of subjects in the older age group N 70-80, possibly slightly lower power. ([ResearchGate https://www.researchgate.net/publication/271673579_Working_memory_across_the_lifespan_A_cross-sectional_approach?utm _source=chatgpt.com])
6 Subsequent research and practical implications
An attempt to simultaneously measure age-related changes and brain structural indices (anterior cingulate cortex thickness and white matter connectivity) in a longitudinal study is underway. ([The Times https://www.thetimes.co.uk/article/friendship-not-fitness-linked-to-sharp-memory-in-later-life-lxkc6xjrl?utm_source= chatgpt.com], ScienceDirect)
In classroom interventions, instructional methods that reduce WM load, such as "diagramming what is heard" and "presenting in short multisensory blocks," are recommended. (Taylor & Francis Online, SpringerOpen https ://ejo.springeropen.com/articles/10.1186/s43163-025-00806-6?utm_source=chatgpt.com)
summary
Alloway & Alloway (2013) simultaneously demonstrated (1) rapid growth in childhood, peak at age 30, and slow decline in old age, and (2) domain-specific structure of WM, making us reconsider the common belief that "aging = rapid decline". Despite cross-sectional limitations, the research continues to have broad implications for education, clinical practice, and neuroscience.
---
@kohaken_dsl: There is a guideline for the number of instructions that can be understood at once, depending on age. Of course, this is just a reference as it varies greatly from person to person, but at 5-6 years old, for example, two instructions are considered the limit.
It is better to refrain from giving too many instructions at once, and to devise a way to say more than three at a time, or to write them down ^ ^.
https://gyazo.com/c05c351077ceec56ef37b5107e278e60
Is this the original paper?
Working memory across the lifespan: A cross-sectional approach src
The aim of the present study was to extend previous lifespan research to a wide age range (5 to 80 year olds) and investigate any potential differences in the development and decline of working memory functions. To that end, measures of both verbal and visuo-spatial working memory were included in a cross-sectional study. The findings indicated that there is considerable growth in childhood–on average 23 standard points; with performance peaking in 30-year olds. There was relatively little change in working memory performance in older adults, with 70 to 80 year olds performing at comparable levels to teenagers (13–19 year olds) in verbal working memory tests. Confirmatory factor analyses suggest that working memory skills across the lifespan are driven by domain differences (i.e., verbal or visuo-spatial stimuli), rather than functional differences (maintenance and manipulation of information).
(DeepL) The purpose of the present study was to extend previous lifespan studies to a broad age range (5 to 80 years) and to investigate potential differences in the development and decline of working memory function. To this end, measures of both verbal working memory and visual and spatial working memory were included in the cross-sectional study. The results revealed that there was substantial growth in childhood, averaging 23 standard points, reaching a peak in the 30s. Older adults showed relatively little change in working memory performance, with 70-80 year olds performing as well as teenagers (13-19 years old) on verbal working memory tests. Confirmatory factor analysis suggested that working memory performance throughout life is driven by domain differences (i.e., verbal or visual/spatial stimuli) rather than functional differences (i.e., information maintenance and manipulation).
Alloway, T.P. and Alloway, R.G., 2013. Working memory across the lifespan: A cross-sectional approach. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(1), pp.84-93.
Age-related changes
---
This page is auto-translated from /nishio/ワーキングメモリは30歳代でピークに達する using DeepL. If you looks something interesting but the auto-translated English is not good enough to understand it, feel free to let me know at @nishio_en. I'm very happy to spread my thought to non-Japanese readers.